Contact information of Cambridge University Press
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help
correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:cup:judgdm. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Kirk Stebbing (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://www.cambridge.org/jdm .
Content
January 2024, Volume 19
- 1-1 Intragroup communication in social dilemmas: An artefactual public good field experiment in small-scale communities
by Hoenow, Nils Christian & Pourviseh, Adrian
- 1-1 Repeated risky choices become more consistent with themselves but not expected value, with no effect of matched trial order
by Spicer, Jake & Mullett, Timothy L. & Sanborn, Adam N.
- 1-1 Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in intertemporal choice
by Burghoorn, Floor & Heuvelmans, Vivian R. & Scheres, Anouk & Roelofs, Karin & Figner, Bernd
- 1-1 The impact of diversity on group decision-making in the face of the free-rider problem
by Stolle, Chris M. & Gula, Bartosz & Yu, Rongjun & Huang, Yi
- 1-1 Choosing, rejecting, and closely replicating, 30 years later: A commentary on Chandrashekar et al
by Shafir, Eldar & Cheek, Nathan N.
- 1-1 The impact of experience on the tendency to accept recommended defaults
by Roth, Yefim & Waldman, Greta Maayan & Erev, Ido
- 1-1 Apocalypse now or later? Nuclear war risk perceptions mirroring media coverage and emotional tone shifts in Italian news
by Lauriola, Marco & Di Cicco, Gabriele & Savadori, Lucia
- 1-1 Eye movements as a tool to investigate exemplar retrieval in judgments
by Rosner, Agnes & Brändli, Fabienne & von Helversen, Bettina
- 1-1 Negative economic shocks and the compliance to social norms
by Bogliacino, Francesco & Charris, Rafael & Gómez, Camilo & Montealegre, Felipe
- 1-1 The Dutch moral foundations stimulus database: An adaptation and validation of moral vignettes and sociomoral images in a Dutch sample
by Hopp, Frederic R. & Jargow, Benjamin & Kouwen, Esmee & Bakker, Bert N.
- 1-1 Factors that promote the repulsion effect in preferential choice
by Banerjee, Pronobesh & Hayes, William M. & Chatterjee, Promothesh & Masters, Tamara & Mishra, Sanjay & Wedell, Douglas H.
- 1-1 Behavioral economics enhancers
by Yechiam, Eldad
- 1-1 A psychological model of collective risk perceptions
by Pirla, Sergio
- 1-1 Likelihood neglect bias and the mental simulations approach: An illustration using the old and new Monty Hall problems
by Wilcox, John E.
- 1-1 Measuring vacillations in reasoning
by Shivnekar, Revati Vijay & Srivastava, Nisheeth
- 1-1 Who is generous and to whom? Generosity among Christians, Muslims, and atheists in the USA, Sweden, Egypt, and Lebanon
by Hallin, Nathalie & Moche, Hajdi & Andersson, Gerhard & Västfjäll, Daniel
- 1-1 What’s moral wiggle room? A theory specification
by Fahrenwaldt, Alina & tho Pesch, Fiona & Fiedler, Susann & Baumert, Anna
- 1-1 Americans believe in the benevolence of nature, and this belief is not lower in people who have experienced natural disasters
by Rozin, Paul & Chen, Richard & Scott, Sydney E. & Cusimano, Corey
- 1-1 The role of game riskiness on the expectation-cooperation link in social dilemmas and its relations with fear and greed
by Ng, Gary Ting Tat & Au, Wing Tung & Lai, Derek Chun Kiu
- 1-1 Sunk cost predictions as theory of mind
by Howard, Amy & Sehl, Claudia & Denison, Stephanie & Friedman, Ori
- 1-1 Evaluating science: A comparison of human and AI reviewers
by Shcherbiak, Anna & Habibnia, Hooman & Böhm, Robert & Fiedler, Susann
- 1-1 In political judgment contrast is stronger than assimilation, especially when polarization is high
by Ganzach, Yoav
- 1-1 The centrality of reasoning in moral judgments: First- and third-party evaluations of cheating
by Waltzer, Tal & Samuelson, Arvid & Dahl, Audun
- 1-1 Can simulated experience be harnessed to help people make investment decisions?
by Lejarraga, Tomás & Ranganathan, Kavitha & Wulff, Dirk U.
- 1-1 Advice taking when the stakes are high: Evidence from a game show
by Løhre, Erik & Halkjelsvik, Torleif
- 1-1 Revisiting the impact of singularity on the Identified Victim Effect: Replication and extension of Kogut and Ritov (2005a) Study 2
by Majumder, Rajarshi & Tai, Yik Long & Ziano, Ignazio & Feldman, Gilad
- 1-1 Individual differences in overconfidence: A new measurement approach
by Binnendyk, Jabin & Pennycook, Gordon
- 1-1 Applying the sour grapes/sweet lemons rationalization to change beliefs and preferences: Reducing public concerns about vaccine safety and enhancing their willingness to get vaccinated
by Du, Si-Yuan & Zhang, Chun & Ma, Jia-Tao & Nharen, Mandura & Dai, Yi-Xin & Li, Pei-Wei & Zhao, Ning & Zheng, Yu & Li, Shu
- 1-1 Assessing patience and predictivity validity for mixed sign intertemporal choices
by Mansell, Wade Sean & Li, Ye & Hardisty, David
- 1-1 Chasing emotional losses: Negative subjective affect is linked to increased risk-seeking behavior both within and between individuals
by Bennett, Daniel & Fulton, Ashleigh & Forbes, Laura
- 1-1 Judgments and beliefs about climate change: measurement, stability, and behavioral consequences
by Hilbig, Benjamin E.
- 1-1 A note on judgments and behavior: Distancing and Corona virus exposure
by Svenson, Ola & Duce Gimeno, Inés & Nilsson, Mats & Salo, Ilkka & Lindholm, Torun
- 1-1 Group collaboration reduces delay discounting of intertemporal choices and its duration
by Geng, Xiaowei & Zhao, Yating & Xu, Shiyuan & Sun, Xinye & Zhou, Xiaolin
- 1-1 Beyond analytic bounds: Re-evaluating predictive power in risky decision models
by Agassi, Or David & Plonsky, Ori
November 2022, Volume 17, Issue 6
- 1176-1207 Pretrial release judgments and decision fatigue
by Shroff, Ravi & Vamvourellis, Konstantinos
- 1208-1228 When and why people perform mindless math
by Lawson, M. Asher & Larrick, Richard P. & Soll, Jack B.
- 1229-1254 Do people believe that you can have too much money? The relationship between hypothetical lottery wins and expected happiness
by Haesevoets, Tessa & Dierckx, Kim & Van Hiel, Alain
- 1255-1286 Successful everyday decision making: Combining attributes and associates
by Banks, Adrian P. & Gamblin, David M.
- 1287-1312 Value-directed information search in partner choice
by Wang, Hongyi & Ma, Jiaxin & He, Lisheng
- 1313-1333 Social preferences before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in China
by King Li, King & Hong, Ying-yi & Huang, Bo & Tam, Tony
- 1335-1352 Scientific contagion heuristic: Judgments about the acceptability of water for religious use after potential scientific treatment
by Mukherjee, Sumitava & Mukherjee, Payel C.
- 1353-1378 Waiting is painful: The impact of anticipated dread on negative discounting in the loss domain
by Sun, Hong-Yue & Ma, Jia-Tao & Zhou, Lei & Jiang, Cheng-Ming & Li, Shu
- 1379-1391 The prominence effect in health-care priority setting
by Persson, Emil & Erlandsson, Arvid & Slovic, Paul & Västfjäll, Daniel & Tinghög, Gustav
- 1392-1421 Hypothesized drivers of the bias blind spot—cognitive sophistication, introspection bias, and conversational processes
by Mandel, David R. & Collins, Robert N. & Walker, Alexander C. & Fugelsang, Jonathan A. & Risko, Evan F.
September 2022, Volume 17, Issue 5
- 937-961 Failing to ignore the ignorant: Mistaking ignorance for error
by Vaz, André & Mata, André
- 962-987 Base rate neglect and conservatism in probabilistic reasoning: Insights from eliciting full distributions
by Howe, Piers Douglas Lionel & Perfors, Andrew & Walker, Bradley & Kashima, Yoshihisa & Fay, Nicolas
- 988-1014 The endowment effect in the future: How time shapes buying and selling prices
by Yamamoto, Shohei & Navarro-Martinez, Daniel
- 1015-1042 Loss aversion (simply) does not materialize for smaller losses
by Zeif, Dana & Yechiam, Eldad
- 1043-1057 On the descriptive value of the reliance on small-samples assumption
by Erev, Ido & Cohen, Doron & Yakobi, Ofir
- 1058-1071 Perception of generosity under matching and rebate subsidies
by Chan, Nathan W. & Knowles, Stephen & Peeters, Ronald & Wolk, Leonard
- 1072-1093 Voting under time pressure
by Alós-Ferrer, Carlos & Garagnani, Michele
- 1094-1122 Does boredom affect economic risk preferences?
by Pirla, Sergio & Navarro-Martinez, Daniel
- 1123-1145 Pledging one’s trustworthiness through gifts: An experiment
by Danese, Giuseppe & Mittone, Luigi
- 1146-1175 Sample decisions with description and experience
by Klingebiel, Ronald & Zhu, Feibai
July 2022, Volume 17, Issue 4
- 646-690 Debiasing System 1: Training favours logical over stereotypical intuiting
by Boissin, Esther & Caparos, Serge & Voudouri, Aikaterini & De Neys, Wim
- 691-719 Drafting strategies in fantasy football: A study of competitive sequential human decision making
by Lee, Michael D. & Liu, Siqi
- 720-744 Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: A meta-analysis
by Yelbuz, Büşra Elif & Madan, Ecesu & Alper, Sinan
- 745-767 Preferences after pan(dem)ics: Time and risk in the shadow of COVID-19
by Gassmann, Xavier & Malézieux, Antoine & Spiegelman, Eli & Tisserand, Jean-Christian
- 768-796 Choosing to choose or not
by Shoval, Roy & Karsh, Noam & Eitam, Baruch
- 797-815 The effect of a reference point in task difficulty: How does a task that becomes irrelevant affect effort, feelings and perceptions
by Voslinsky, Alisa & Azar, Ofer H.
- 816-848 The advice less taken: The consequences of receiving unexpected advice
by Rebholz, Tobias R. & Hütter, Mandy
- 849-882 The effects of communicating scientific uncertainty on trust and decision making in a public health context
by Schneider, Claudia R. & Freeman, Alexandra L. J. & Spiegelhalter, David & van der Linden, Sander
- 883-936 Stress and risk — Preferences versus noise
by Parslow, Elle & Rose, Julia
May 2022, Volume 17, Issue 3
- 487-512 Explaining human sampling rates across different decision domains
by van de Wouw, Didrika S. & McKay, Ryan T. & Averbeck, Bruno B. & Furl, Nicholas
- 513-546 The day after the disaster: Risk-taking following large- and small-scale disasters in a microworld
by Liang, Garston & Rakow, Tim & Yechiam, Eldad & Newell, Ben R.
- 547-573 Susceptibility to misinformation is consistent across question framings and response modes and better explained by myside bias and partisanship than analytical thinking
by Roozenbeek, Jon & Maertens, Rakoen & Herzog, Stefan M. & Geers, Michael & Kurvers, Ralf & Sultan, Mubashir & van der Linden, Sander
- 574-597 Maximize when valuable: The domain specificity of maximizing decision-making style
by Zhu, Minfan & Wang, Jun & Xie, Xiaofei
- 598-627 Combining white box models, black box machines and human interventions for interpretable decision strategies
by Gadzinski, Gregory & Castello, Alessio
- 628-645 Expectations of how machines use individuating information and base-rates
by English, Sarah D. & Denison, Stephanie & Friedman, Ori
March 2022, Volume 17, Issue 2
- 237-262 Preference for playing order in games with and without replacement: Motivational biases and probability misestimations
by Suk, Kwanho & Koo, Jieun
- 263-283 “When in Rome”: Identifying social norms using coordination games
by Krupka, Erin L. & Weber, Roberto & Crosno, Rachel T. A. & Hoover, Hanna
- 284-314 Testing team reasoning: Group identification is related to coordination in pure coordination games
by Thom, James Matthew & Afzal, Uzma & Gold, Natalie
- 315-330 Does the evaluability bias hold when giving to animal charities?
by Spiteri, Glen William
- 331-361 Cognitive miserliness in argument literacy? Effects of intuitive and analytic thinking on recognizing fallacies
by Svedholm-Häkkinen, Annika M. & Kiikeri, Mika
- 362-377 Belief in karma is associated with perceived (but not actual) trustworthiness
by Ong, How Hwee & Evans, Anthony M. & Nelissen, Rob M. A. & van Beest, Ilja
- 378-399 Effects of icon arrays to communicate risk in a repeated risky decision-making task
by Price, Paul C. & Carlock, Grace A. & Crouse, Sarah & Arciga, Mariana Vargas
- 400-424 Pseudocontingencies: Flexible contingency inferences from base rates
by Vogel, Tobias & Ingendahl, Moritz & McCaughey, Linda
- 425-448 Violations of economic rationality due to irrelevant information during learning in decision from experience
by Spektor, Mikhail S. & Seidler, Hannah
January 2022, Volume 17, Issue 2
January 2022, Volume 17, Issue 1
- 1-13 Affect and prosocial behavior: The role of decision mode and individual processing style
by Gärtner, Manja & Andersson, David & Västfjäll, Daniel & Tinghög, Gustav
- 14-30 Thinking, good and bad? Deliberative thinking and the singularity effect in charitable giving
by Moche, Hajdi & Gordon-Hecker, Tom & Kogut, Tehila & Västfjäll, Daniel
- 31-49 Assessing the test-retest reliability of the social value orientation slider measure
by de Matos Fernandes, Carlos A. & Bakker, Dieko M. & Dijkstra, Jacob
- 50-69 Playing with words: Do people exploit loaded language to affect others’ decisions for their own benefit?
by Capraro, Valerio & Vanzo, Andrea & Cabrales, Antonio
- 70-90 Context-dependent outcome expectation contributes to experience-based risky choice
by He, Zhijian & Dai, Junyi
- 91-123 Recalibrating probabilistic forecasts to improve their accuracy
by Han, Ying & Budescu, David V.
- 124-163 Outcome feedback reduces over-forecasting of inflation and overconfidence in forecasts
by Niu, Xiaoxiao & Harvey, Nigel
- 164-188 What drives opposition to suicide? Two exploratory studies of normative judgments
by Landy, Justin F. & Shah, Pritika
- 189-214 After the virtual flood: Risk perceptions and flood preparedness after virtual reality risk communication
by Mol, Jantsje M. & Botzen, W. J. Wouter & Blasch, Julia E.
- 215-236 Frequency or total number? A comparison of different presentation formats on risk perception during COVID-19
by Jie, Yun
November 2021, Volume 16, Issue 6
- 1324-1369 Establishing the laws of preferential choice behavior
by Bhatia, Sudeep & Loomes, Graham & Read, Daniel
- 1370-1391 Expert and novice sensitivity to environmental regularities in predicting NFL games
by Montgomery, Lauren E. & Lee, Michael D.
- 1392-1412 Agency and self-other asymmetries in perceived bias and shortcomings: Replications of the Bias Blind Spot and link to free will beliefs
by Chandrashekar, Subramanya Prasad & Yeung, Siu Kit & Yau, Ka Chai & Cheung, Chung Yee & Agarwal, Tanay Kulbhushan & Wong, Cho Yan Joan & Pillai, Tanishka & Thirlwell, Thea Natasha & Leung, Wing Nam & Tse, Colman & Li, Yan Tung & Cheng, Bo Ley & Chan, Hill Yan Cedar & Feldman, Gilad
- 1413-1438 Development of decision making based on internal and external information: A hierarchical Bayesian approach
by Zadelaar, Jacqueline N. & Rentergem, Joost A. Agelink van & Schaaf, Jessica V. & Dekkers, Tycho J. & Vent, Nathalie de & Dekkers, Laura M. S. & Olthof, Maria C. & Jansen, Brenda R. J. & Huizenga, Hilde M.
- 1439-1463 Success stories cause false beliefs about success
by Lifchits, George & Anderson, Ashton & Goldstein, Daniel G. & Hofman, Jake M. & Watts, Duncan J.
- 1464-1484 Incentivized and non-incentivized liking ratings outperform willingness-to-pay in predicting choice
by Hascher, Joshua & Desai, Nitisha & Krajbich, Ian
- 1485-1505 Webcam-based online eye-tracking for behavioral research
by Yang, Xiaozhi & Krajbich, Ian
- 1506-1524 Differences in cooperation between social dilemmas of gain and loss
by Sun, Qingzhou & Guo, Haozhi & Wang, Jiarui & Zhang, Jing & Jiang, Chengming & Liu, Yongfang
- 1525-1548 Revenge is not blind: Testing the ability of retribution to justify dishonesty
by Peleg, Dar & Hochman, Guy & Levine, Timothy & Klar, Yechiel & Ayal, Shahar
- 1549-1574 A brief forewarning intervention overcomes negative effects of salient changes in COVID-19 guidance
by Gretton, Jeremy D. & Meyers, Ethan A. & Walker, Alexander C. & Fugelsang, Jonathan A. & Koehler, Derek J.
- 1575-1596 COVID-19 as infodemic: The impact of political orientation and open-mindedness on the discernment of misinformation in WhatsApp
by Bonafé-Pontes, Andressa & Couto, Cleno & Kakinohana, Regis & Travain, Mariana & Schimidt, Luísa & Pilati, Ronaldo
September 2021, Volume 16, Issue 5
- 1113-1154 Seven (weak and strong) helping effects systematically tested in separate evaluation, joint evaluation and forced choice
by Erlandsson, Arvid
- 1155-1185 Multiattribute judgment: Acceptance of a new COVID-19 vaccine as a function of price, risk, and effectiveness
by Birnbaum, Michael H.
- 1186-1220 Consumers’ ability to identify a surplus when returns to attributes are nonlinear
by Lunn, Peter D. & Somerville, Jason
- 1221-1233 Input-dependent noise can explain magnitude-sensitivity in optimal value-based decision-making
by Pirrone, Angelo & Reina, Andreagiovanni & Gobet, Fernand
- 1234-1266 Judging fast and slow: The truth effect does not increase under time-pressure conditions
by Nadarevic, Lena & Schnuerch, Martin & Stegemann, Marlena J.
- 1267-1289 Norm shifts under the strategy method
by Columbus, Simon & Böhm, Robert
- 1290-1313 To pay or not to pay: Measuring risk preferences in lab and field
by Brañas-Garza, Pablo & Estepa-Mohedano, Lorenzo & Jorrat, Diego & Orozco, Victor & Rascón-Ramírez, Ericka
- 1314-1323 Risky choice framing by experience: A methodological note
by Kühberger, Anton
July 2021, Volume 16, Issue 4
- 796-822 Asymmetry and symmetry of acts and omissions in punishment, norms, and judged causality
by Handfield, Toby & Thrasher, John & Corcoran, Andrew & Nichols, Shaun
- 823-843 No effects of synchronicity in online social dilemma experiments: A registered report
by Evans, Anthony M. & Kogler, Christoph & Sleegers, Willem W. A.
- 844-897 A hierarchy of mindreading strategies in joint action participation
by Landes, Todd Larson & Howe, Piers Douglas & Kashima, Yoshihisa
- 898-931 Perspective neglect: Inadequate perspective taking limits coordination
by Williams, Elanor F. & Lieberman, Alicea & Amir, On
- 932-949 How economic success shapes redistribution: The role of self-serving beliefs, in-group bias and justice principles
by Dorin, Camille & Hainguerlot, Marine & Huber-Yahi, Hélène & Vergnaud, Jean-Christophe & de Gardelle, Vincent
- 950-968 The relation between disgust sensitivity and risk-taking propensity: A domain specific approach
by Sevi, Barış & Shook, Natalie J.
- 969-1009 Eye-tracking evidence for fixation asymmetries in verbal and numerical quantifier processing
by Holford, Dawn Liu & Juanchich, Marie & Foulsham, Tom & Sirota, Miroslav & Clarke, Alasdair D. F.
- 1010-1038 Visual attention and time preference reversals
by Zhou, Yan-Bang & Li, Qiang & Liu, Hong-Zhi
- 1039-1059 Patterns in manually selected numbers in the Israeli lottery
by Polin, Brian A. & Isaac, Eyal Ben & Aharon, Itzhak
- 1060-1071 Number preferences in selected Nigerian lottery games
by Otekunrin, Oluwaseun A. & Folorunso, Adesola G. & Alawode, Kehinde O.
- 1072-1096 Apparent age and gender differences in survival optimism: To what extent are they a bias in the translation of beliefs onto a percentage scale?
by Comerford, David A.
- 1097-1112 Does pain make people short-sighted? The impacts of physical and psychological pains on intertemporal choice
by Chen, Jing & He, Zhican & Sun, Yue & Xia, Weihai & Liao, Zongqing & Yuan, Jiaqi
May 2021, Volume 16, Issue 3
- 596-613 Numbers do not add up! The pragmatic approach to the framing of medical treatments
by Macchi, Laura & Zulato, Edoardo
- 614-637 The effect of autism on information sampling during decision-making: An eye-tracking study
by Farmer, George D. & Smith, Paula & Baron-Cohen, Simon & Skylark, William J.
- 638-686 More is easier? Testing the role of fluency in the more-credible effect
by Skylark, William J.
- 687-708 Misjudgment of interrupted time-series graphs due to serial dependence: Replication of Matyas and Greenwood (1990)
by Bishara, Anthony J. & Peller, Jacob & Galuska, Chad M.
- 709-728 Comparing mixed intertemporal tradeoffs with pure gains or pure losses
by Ma, Jia-Tao & Wang, Lei & Chen, Li-Na & He, Quan & Sun, Qing-Zhou & Sun, Hong-Yue & Jiang, Cheng-Ming
- 729-742 Influence of divided attention on the attraction effect in multialternative choice
by Tsuzuki, Takashi & Takeda, Yuji & Chiba, Itsuki
- 743-766 Does a second offer that becomes irrelevant affect fairness perceptions and willingness to accept in the ultimatum game?
by Voslinsky, Alisa & Lahav, Yaron & Azar, Ofer H.
- 766-795 Turning up the heat: The impact of indoor temperature on selected cognitive processes and the validity of self-report
by Stroom, Martijn & Kok, Nils & Strobel, Martin & Eichholtz, Piet M. A.
March 2021, Volume 16, Issue 2
- 238-266 Selection effects on dishonest behavior
by Houdek, Petr & Bahník, Štěpán & Hudík, Marek & Vranka, Marek
- 267-289 Underweighting of rare events in social interactions and its implications to the design of voluntary health applications
by Plonsky, Ori & Roth, Yefim & Erev, Ido
- 290-322 Social mindfulness is normative when costs are low, but rapidly declines with increases in costs
by Engel, Christoph & Van Lange, Paul A. M.
- 323-362 Forecasting forecaster accuracy: Contributions of past performance and individual differences
by Himmelstein, Mark & Atanasov, Pavel & Budescu, David V.
- 363-393 Facilitating sender-receiver agreement in communicated probabilities: Is it best to use words, numbers or both?
by Mandel, David R. & Irwin, Daniel
- 394-421 Risky choice framing with various problem descriptions: A replication and extension study
by Zhou, Lei & Liu, Nan & Liao, Ya-Qiong & Li, Ai-Mei
- 422-459 Inference and preference in intertemporal choice
by Skylark, William J. & Farmer, George D. & Bahemia, Nadia
- 460-483 Judgments of frequency and duration: One or two underlying dimensions?
by Titz, Johannes & Sedlmeier, Peter
- 484-504 Beyond “fake news”: Analytic thinking and the detection of false and hyperpartisan news headlines
by Ross, Robert M. & Rand, David G. & Pennycook, Gordon
- 505-550 Denotative and connotative management of uncertainty: A computational dual-process model
by Hoey, Jesse & MacKinnon, Neil J. & Schröder, Tobias
- 551-565 Nudging freelance professionals to increase their retirement pension fund contributions
by Rubaltelli, Enrico & Lotto, Lorella
- 566-595 Consequences, norms, and inaction: Response to Gawronski et al. (2020)
by Baron, Jonathan & Goodwin, Geoffrey P.
January 2021, Volume 16, Issue 1
- 1-19 Veil-of-ignorance reasoning mitigates self-serving bias in resource allocation during the COVID-19 crisis
by Huang, Karen & Bernhard, Regan M. & Barak-Corren, Netta & Bazerman, Max H. & Greene, Joshua D.
- 20-35 Compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: Active vs. passive risk takers
by Keinan, Ruty & Idan, Tali & Bereby-Meyer, Yoella
- 36-56 Accentuation and compatibility: Replication and extensions of Shafir (1993) to rethink choosing versus rejecting paradigms
by Chandrashekar, Subramanya Prasad & Weber, Jasmin & Chan, Sze Ying & Cho, Won Young & Chu, Tsz Ching Connie & Cheng, Bo Ley & Feldman, Gilad
- 57-93 Attentional shifts and preference reversals: An eye-tracking study
by Alós-Ferrer, Carlos & Jaudas, Alexander & Ritschel, Alexander
- 94-113 When two wrongs make a right: The efficiency-consumption gap under separate vs. joint evaluations
by Gamliel, Eyal & Pe’er, Eyal
- 114-130 Myopia drives reckless behavior in response to over-taxation
by Spektor, Mikhail S. & Wulff, Dirk U.
- 131-141 Anchoring without scale distortion
by Bahník, Štěpán
- 142-164 Steady steps versus sudden shifts: Cooperation in (a)symmetric linear and step-level social dilemmas
by Kas, Judith & Hardisty, David J. & Handgraaf, Michel J. J.
- 165-200 The effects of tool comparisons when estimating the likelihood of task success
by Li, Shuqi & Miller, Jane E. & O’Rourke Stuart, Jillian & Jules, Sean J. & Scherer, Aaron M. & Smith, Andrew R. & Windschitl, Paul D.
- 201-237 How preference change induced by mere action versus inaction persists over time
by Chen, Zhang & Holland, Rob W. & Quandt, Julian & Dijksterhuis, Ap & Veling, Harm
November 2020, Volume 15, Issue 6
- 881-888 Without a mask: Judgments of Corona virus exposure as a function of inter personal distance
by Svenson, Ola & Appelbom, Sophia & Mayorga, Marcus & Lindholm Öjmyr, Torun
- 889-908 “Quick and dirty”: Intuitive cognitive style predicts trust in Didier Raoult and his hydroxychloroquine-based treatment against COVID-19
by Fuhrer, Joffrey & Cova, Florian
- 909-925 Inducing feelings of ignorance makes people more receptive to expert (economist) opinion
by Meyers, Ethan A. & Turpin, Martin H. & Białek, Michał & Fugelsang, Jonathan A. & Koehler, Derek J.
- 926-938 Activating reflective thinking with decision justification and debiasing training
by Isler, Ozan & Yilmaz, Onurcan & Dogruyol, Burak
- 939-958 Coherence of probability judgments from uncertain evidence: Does ACH help?
by Karvetski, Christopher W. & Mandel, David R.
- 959-971 Will she give you two cookies for one chocolate? Children’s intuitions about trades
by Echelbarger, Margaret & Good, Kayla & Shaw, Alex
- 972-988 Kilo what? Default units increase value sensitivity in joint evaluations of energy efficiency
by Herberz, Mario & Brosch, Tobias & Hahnel, Ulf J. J.
- 989-993 Unit Asking — a method for increasing donations: A replication and extension
by Karlsson, Hulda & Hellström, Simon & Moche, Hajdi & Västfjäll, Daniel
- 994-1008 More time, more work: How time limits bias estimates of task scope and project duration
by Goswami, Indranil & Urminsky, Oleg
- 1009-1023 Carryover of domain-dependent risk preferences in a novel decision-making task
by Shapiro, Martin S. & Price, Paul C. & Mitchell, Edward
- 1024-1036 A meta-analytical and experimental examination of blood glucose effects on decision making under risk
by Orquin, Jacob Lund & Christensen, Jacob Dalgaard & Lagerkvist, Carl-Johan
- 1037-1043 Evaluating prosocial COVID-19 messaging frames: Evidence from a field study on Facebook
by Banker, Sachin & Park, Joowon
- 1044-1051 Reanalysis of Butler and Pogrebna (2018) using true and error model
by Birnbaum, Michael H.
- 1052-1053 Intransitive preferences or choice errors? A reply to Birnbaum
by Butler, David
- 1054-1072 On the validity of the CNI model of moral decision-making: Reply to Baron and Goodwin (2020)
by Gawronski, Bertram & Conway, Paul & Hütter, Mandy & Luke, Dillon M. & Armstrong, Joel & Friesdorf, Rebecca
September 2020, Volume 15, Issue 5
- 611-629 The delay-reward heuristic: What do people expect in intertemporal choice tasks?
by Skylark, William J. & Chan, Kieran T. F. & Farmer, George D. & Gaskin, Kai W. & Miller, Amelia R.
- 630-647 Mycological rationality: Heuristics, perception and decision-making in mushroom foraging
by Kaaronen, Roope O.
- 648-659 Strategic thinking and behavior during a pandemic
by Halevy, Nir
- 660-684 Comparing fast thinking and slow thinking: The relative benefits of interventions, individual differences, and inferential rules
by Lawson, M. Asher & Larrick, Richard P. & Soll, Jack B.
- 685-703 Risky choice frames shift the structure and emotional valence of internal arguments: A query theory account of the unusual disease problem
by Wall, Daniel & Crookes, Raymond D. & Johnson, Eric J. & Weber, Elke U.
- 704-726 Attraction comes from many sources: Attentional and comparative processes in decoy effects
by Marini, Marco & Ansani, Alessandro & Paglieri, Fabio
- 727-740 Too smart for their own good: Trading truthfulness for efficiency in the Israeli medical internship market
by Rosenfeld, Ariel & Hassidim, Avinatan
- 741-755 A reflection on cognitive reflection – testing convergent/divergent validity of two measures of cognitive reflection
by Erceg, Nikola & Galić, Zvonimir & Ružojčić, Mitja
- 756-762 Impact of superstitious beliefs on the timing of marriage and childbirth: Evidence from Denmark
by Antipov, Evgeny A. & Pokryshevskaya, Elena B.
- 783-797 Effect of confidence interval construction on judgment accuracy
by Mandel, David R. & Collins, Robert N. & Risko, Evan F. & Fugelsang, Jonathan A.
- 798-806 The way of making choices: Maximizing and satisficing and its relationship to well-being, personality, and self-rumination
by Vargová, Lenka & Zibrínová, Ľubica & Baník, Gabriel
- 807-822 Does pregnancy make women more cautious and calm? The impact of pregnancy on risk decision-making
by Chen, Jing & Guo, Yinghan & Liao, Zongqing & Xia, Weihai & She, Shengxiang
- 823-850 A cognitive modeling analysis of risk in sequential choice tasks
by Guan, Maime & Stokes, Ryan & Vandekerckhove, Joachim & Lee, Michael D.
- 851-861 The individual true and error model: Getting the most out of limited data
by Schramm, Pele
- 861-862 Note on Birnbaum and Wan (2020): True and error model analysis is robust with respect to certain violations of the MARTER model
by Birnbaum, Michael H. & Quan, Bonny
- 863-880 Information, incentives, and goals in election forecasts
by Gelman, Andrew & Hullman, Jessica & Wlezien, Christopher & Elliott Morris, George
July 2020, Volume 15, Issue 4
- 452-475 Moral preferences in helping dilemmas expressed by matching and forced choice
by Erlandsson, Arvid & Lindkvist, Amanda & Lundqvist, Kajsa & Andersson, Per A. & Dickert, Stephan & Slovic, Paul & Västfjäll, Daniel
- 476-498 On the belief that beliefs should change according to evidence: Implications for conspiratorial, moral, paranormal, political, religious, and science beliefs
by Pennycook, Gordon & Cheyne, James Allan & Koehler, Derek J. & Fugelsang, Jonathan A.
- 499-508 Taking risks for the best: Maximizing and risk-taking tendencies
by Qiu, Tian & Bai, Yang & Lu, Jingyi
- 509-516 Donors vastly underestimate differences in charities’ effectiveness
by Caviola, Lucius & Schubert, Stefan & Teperman, Elliot & Moss, David & Greenberg, Spencer & Faber, Nadira S.
- 517-533 Procedural and economic utilities in consequentialist choice: Trading freedom of choice to minimize financial losses
by DeCaro, Daniel A. & DeCaro, Marci S. & Hotaling, Jared M. & Johnson, Joseph G.
- 534-544 Gender differences in the trade-off between objective equality and efficiency
by Capraro, Valerio
- 545-560 Biased perceptions about momentum: Do comeback teams have higher chances to win in basketball overtimes?
by Morgulev, Elia & Voslinsky, Alisa & Azar, Ofer H. & Bar-Eli, Michael
- 561-571 Solve the dilemma by spinning a penny? On using random decision-making aids
by Jaffé, Mariela E. & Douneva, Maria & Greifeneder, Rainer
- 572-585 Are the symptoms really remitting? How the subjective interpretation of outcomes can produce an illusion of causality
by Blanco, Fernando & Moreno-Fernández, Maria Manuela & Matute, Helena
- 586-599 The psychology of task management: The smaller tasks trap
by Rusou, Zohar & Amar, Moty & Ayal, Shahar
- 600-610 May the odds — or your personality — be in your favor: Probability of observing a favorable outcome, Honesty-Humility, and dishonest behavior
by Schild, Christoph & Moshagen, Morten & Ścigała, Karolina A. & Zettler, Ingo
May 2020, Volume 15, Issue 3
- 304-310 Money makes the world go round, and basic research can help
by Erev, Ido
- 312-329 An experimental guide to vehicles in the park
by Struchiner, Noel & Hannikainen, Ivar R. & de Almeida, Guilherme da F. C. F.
- 330-345 Do multiple-trial games better reflect prosocial behavior than single-trial games?
by Haesevoets, Tessa & Van Hiel, Alain & Dierckx, Kim & Reinders Folmer, Chris
- 346-352 Crowding-out (-in) Effects of Subsidy Schemes on Individual Donations: An Experimental Study
by Peng, Hui-Chun & Liu, Wen-Jing
- 353-370 Harbingers of foul play: A field study of gain/loss frames and regulatory fit in the NFL
by Polman, Evan & Van Swol, Lyn M. & Hoban, Paul R.
- 371-380 Inducing alternative-based and characteristic-based search procedures in risky choice
by Mittone, Luigi & Papi, Mauro
- 381-400 Delay discounting and risky choice: Meta-analytic evidence regarding single-process theories
by Johnson, Kelli L. & Bixter, Michael T. & Luhmann, Christian C.