IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/plo/pone00/0165932.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Binocular Depth Judgments on Smoothly Curved Surfaces

Author

Listed:
  • Rebecca L Hornsey
  • Paul B Hibbard
  • Peter Scarfe

Abstract

Binocular disparity is an important cue to depth, allowing us to make very fine discriminations of the relative depth of objects. In complex scenes, this sensitivity depends on the particular shape and layout of the objects viewed. For example, judgments of the relative depths of points on a smoothly curved surface are less accurate than those for points in empty space. It has been argued that this occurs because depth relationships are represented accurately only within a local spatial area. A consequence of this is that, when judging the relative depths of points separated by depth maxima and minima, information must be integrated across separate local representations. This integration, by adding more stages of processing, might be expected to reduce the accuracy of depth judgements. We tested this idea directly by measuring how accurately human participants could report the relative depths of two dots, presented with different binocular disparities. In the first, Two Dot condition the two dots were presented in front of a square grid. In the second, Three Dot condition, an additional dot was presented midway between the target dots, at a range of depths, both nearer and further than the target dots. In the final, Surface condition, the target dots were placed on a smooth surface defined by binocular disparity cues. In some trials, this contained a depth maximum or minimum between the target dots. In the Three Dot condition, performance was impaired when the central dot was presented with a large disparity, in line with predictions. In the Surface condition, performance was worst when the midpoint of the surface was at a similar distance to the targets, and relatively unaffected when there was a large depth maximum or minimum present. These results are not consistent with the idea that depth order is represented only within a local spatial area.

Suggested Citation

  • Rebecca L Hornsey & Paul B Hibbard & Peter Scarfe, 2016. "Binocular Depth Judgments on Smoothly Curved Surfaces," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 11(11), pages 1-18, November.
  • Handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0165932
    DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165932
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0165932
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0165932&type=printable
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1371/journal.pone.0165932?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. B. G. Cumming & A. J. Parker, 1997. "Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception," Nature, Nature, vol. 389(6648), pages 280-283, September.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Maria Solé Puig & Laura Pérez Zapata & J Antonio Aznar-Casanova & Hans Supèr, 2013. "A Role of Eye Vergence in Covert Attention," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 8(1), pages 1-10, January.
    2. Antoine Barbot & Anasuya Das & Michael D. Melnick & Matthew R. Cavanaugh & Elisha P. Merriam & David J. Heeger & Krystel R. Huxlin, 2021. "Spared perilesional V1 activity underlies training-induced recovery of luminance detection sensitivity in cortically-blind patients," Nature Communications, Nature, vol. 12(1), pages 1-18, December.
    3. Gross, Eitan, 2015. "Classification error analysis in stereo vision," Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, Elsevier, vol. 430(C), pages 1-10.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0165932. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: plosone (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/ .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.