IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/beheco/v24y2013i5p1114-1121..html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Tadpole antipredator responses change over time: what is the role of learning and generalization?

Author

Listed:
  • Douglas P. Chivers
  • Maud C.O. Ferrari

Abstract

Many prey animals quickly learn the identity and risk level associated with unknown predators and this provides them with an immediate survival advantage. However, as time passes the information they learned about the risk level associated with the predator decreases in value. As the certainty of the information decreases, there should be a point in time when the prey should stop responding to the information. Here, we conditioned tadpoles to recognize a tiger salamander, fire belly newt, or goldfish and found that they responded to each of the 3 predators with an equal response both 1 day and 8 days postconditioning. Subsequently, we reconditioned each of the groups to recognize tiger salamanders alone and found that the duration of time for which the tadpoles responded to the tiger salamander cue was influenced by what the tadpoles learned in the past. Tadpoles conditioned twice to the tiger salamander retained their response the longest, whereas tadpoles taught goldfish and then tiger salamander responded to the tiger salamander for the least time. Those learning the newt and then the tiger salamander retained their response to the tiger salamander for an intermediate amount of time, indicating that information gained through predator generalization influences the retention of responses to predators. Our results highlight the complex algorithm used by animals to acquire, encode, and use information from their environment.

Suggested Citation

  • Douglas P. Chivers & Maud C.O. Ferrari, 2013. "Tadpole antipredator responses change over time: what is the role of learning and generalization?," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(5), pages 1114-1121.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:24:y:2013:i:5:p:1114-1121.
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/art038
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:24:y:2013:i:5:p:1114-1121.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.