IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/nat/nature/v615y2023i7951d10.1038_s41586-023-05748-8.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Cardiogenic control of affective behavioural state

Author

Listed:
  • Brian Hsueh

    (Stanford University)

  • Ritchie Chen

    (Stanford University)

  • YoungJu Jo

    (Stanford University)

  • Daniel Tang

    (Stanford University)

  • Misha Raffiee

    (Stanford University)

  • Yoon Seok Kim

    (Stanford University)

  • Masatoshi Inoue

    (Stanford University)

  • Sawyer Randles

    (Stanford University)

  • Charu Ramakrishnan

    (Stanford University)

  • Sneha Patel

    (Stanford University)

  • Doo Kyung Kim

    (Stanford University)

  • Tony X. Liu

    (Stanford University)

  • Soo Hyun Kim

    (Stanford University)

  • Longzhi Tan

    (Stanford University)

  • Leili Mortazavi

    (Stanford University)

  • Arjay Cordero

    (Stanford University)

  • Jenny Shi

    (Stanford University)

  • Mingming Zhao

    (Stanford University)

  • Theodore T. Ho

    (Stanford University)

  • Ailey Crow

    (Stanford University)

  • Ai-Chi Wang Yoo

    (Stanford University)

  • Cephra Raja

    (Stanford University)

  • Kathryn Evans

    (Stanford University)

  • Daniel Bernstein

    (Stanford University)

  • Michael Zeineh

    (Stanford University)

  • Maged Goubran

    (Stanford University)

  • Karl Deisseroth

    (Stanford University
    Stanford University
    Stanford University)

Abstract

Emotional states influence bodily physiology, as exemplified in the top-down process by which anxiety causes faster beating of the heart1–3. However, whether an increased heart rate might itself induce anxiety or fear responses is unclear3–8. Physiological theories of emotion, proposed over a century ago, have considered that in general, there could be an important and even dominant flow of information from the body to the brain9. Here, to formally test this idea, we developed a noninvasive optogenetic pacemaker for precise, cell-type-specific control of cardiac rhythms of up to 900 beats per minute in freely moving mice, enabled by a wearable micro-LED harness and the systemic viral delivery of a potent pump-like channelrhodopsin. We found that optically evoked tachycardia potently enhanced anxiety-like behaviour, but crucially only in risky contexts, indicating that both central (brain) and peripheral (body) processes may be involved in the development of emotional states. To identify potential mechanisms, we used whole-brain activity screening and electrophysiology to find brain regions that were activated by imposed cardiac rhythms. We identified the posterior insular cortex as a potential mediator of bottom-up cardiac interoceptive processing, and found that optogenetic inhibition of this brain region attenuated the anxiety-like behaviour that was induced by optical cardiac pacing. Together, these findings reveal that cells of both the body and the brain must be considered together to understand the origins of emotional or affective states. More broadly, our results define a generalizable approach for noninvasive, temporally precise functional investigations of joint organism-wide interactions among targeted cells during behaviour.

Suggested Citation

  • Brian Hsueh & Ritchie Chen & YoungJu Jo & Daniel Tang & Misha Raffiee & Yoon Seok Kim & Masatoshi Inoue & Sawyer Randles & Charu Ramakrishnan & Sneha Patel & Doo Kyung Kim & Tony X. Liu & Soo Hyun Kim, 2023. "Cardiogenic control of affective behavioural state," Nature, Nature, vol. 615(7951), pages 292-299, March.
  • Handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:615:y:2023:i:7951:d:10.1038_s41586-023-05748-8
    DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-05748-8
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41586-023-05748-8
    File Function: Abstract
    Download Restriction: Access to the full text of the articles in this series is restricted.

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1038/s41586-023-05748-8?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:615:y:2023:i:7951:d:10.1038_s41586-023-05748-8. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.