IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/nat/nature/v393y1998i6686d10.1038_31489.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Premotor commands encode monocular eye movements

Author

Listed:
  • Wu Zhou

    (University of Mississippi Medical Center)

  • W. M. King

    (University of Mississippi Medical Center)

Abstract

Binocular coordination of eye movements is essential for stereopsis (depth perception) and to prevent double vision. More than a century ago, Hering and Helmholtz debated the neural basis of binocular coordination. Helmholtz1 believed that each eye is controlled independently and that binocular coordination is learned. Hering2 believed that both eyes are innervated by common command signals that yoke the eye movements (Hering's law of equal innervation). Here we provide evidence that Hering's law is unlikely to be correct. We show that premotor neurons in the paramedian pontine reticular formation that were thought to encode conjugate3,4,5,6 velocity commands for saccades (rapid eye movements) actually encode monocular commands for either right or left eye saccades. However, 66% of the abducens motor neurons, which innervate the ipsilateral lateral rectus muscle, fire as a result of movements of either eye. The distribution of sensitivity to ipsilateral and contralateral eye movements across the abducens motor neuron pool may provide a basis for learning binocular coordination in infancy and adapting it throughout life.

Suggested Citation

  • Wu Zhou & W. M. King, 1998. "Premotor commands encode monocular eye movements," Nature, Nature, vol. 393(6686), pages 692-695, June.
  • Handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:393:y:1998:i:6686:d:10.1038_31489
    DOI: 10.1038/31489
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://www.nature.com/articles/31489
    File Function: Abstract
    Download Restriction: Access to the full text of the articles in this series is restricted.

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1038/31489?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:393:y:1998:i:6686:d:10.1038_31489. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.