IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/nat/nature/v626y2024i8001d10.1038_s41586-023-07008-1.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

A distinct cortical code for socially learned threat

Author

Listed:
  • Shana E. Silverstein

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Ruairi O’Sullivan

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Olena Bukalo

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Dipanwita Pati

    (University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill)

  • Julia A. Schaffer

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Aaron Limoges

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Leo Zsembik

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Takayuki Yoshida

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • John J. O’Malley

    (National Institute of Mental Health, NIH)

  • Ronald F. Paletzki

    (National Institute of Mental Health, NIH)

  • Abby G. Lieberman

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Mio Nonaka

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

  • Karl Deisseroth

    (Stanford University
    Stanford University
    Stanford University)

  • Charles R. Gerfen

    (National Institute of Mental Health, NIH)

  • Mario A. Penzo

    (National Institute of Mental Health, NIH)

  • Thomas L. Kash

    (University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill)

  • Andrew Holmes

    (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH)

Abstract

Animals can learn about sources of danger while minimizing their own risk by observing how others respond to threats. However, the distinct neural mechanisms by which threats are learned through social observation (known as observational fear learning1–4 (OFL)) to generate behavioural responses specific to such threats remain poorly understood. The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) performs several key functions that may underlie OFL, including processing of social information and disambiguation of threat cues5–11. Here we show that dmPFC is recruited and required for OFL in mice. Using cellular-resolution microendoscopic calcium imaging, we demonstrate that dmPFC neurons code for observational fear and do so in a manner that is distinct from direct experience. We find that dmPFC neuronal activity predicts upcoming switches between freezing and moving state elicited by threat. By combining neuronal circuit mapping, calcium imaging, electrophysiological recordings and optogenetics, we show that dmPFC projections to the midbrain periaqueductal grey (PAG) constrain observer freezing, and that amygdalar and hippocampal inputs to dmPFC opposingly modulate observer freezing. Together our findings reveal that dmPFC neurons compute a distinct code for observational fear and coordinate long-range neural circuits to select behavioural responses.

Suggested Citation

  • Shana E. Silverstein & Ruairi O’Sullivan & Olena Bukalo & Dipanwita Pati & Julia A. Schaffer & Aaron Limoges & Leo Zsembik & Takayuki Yoshida & John J. O’Malley & Ronald F. Paletzki & Abby G. Lieberma, 2024. "A distinct cortical code for socially learned threat," Nature, Nature, vol. 626(8001), pages 1066-1072, February.
  • Handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:626:y:2024:i:8001:d:10.1038_s41586-023-07008-1
    DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-07008-1
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41586-023-07008-1
    File Function: Abstract
    Download Restriction: Access to the full text of the articles in this series is restricted.

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1038/s41586-023-07008-1?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:626:y:2024:i:8001:d:10.1038_s41586-023-07008-1. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.