IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/beheco/v15y2004i6p1023-1030.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits

Author

Listed:
  • Niels Jeroen Dingemanse
  • Piet de Goede

Abstract

Individual differences in personality affect behavior in novel or challenging situations. Personality traits may be subject to selection because they affect the ability to dominate others. We investigated whether dominance rank at feeding tables in winter correlated with a heritable personality trait (as measured by exploratory behavior in a novel environment) in a natural population of great tits, Parus major. We provided clumped resources at feeding tables and calculated linear dominance hierarchies on the basis of observations between dyads of color-ringed individuals, and we used an experimental procedure to measure individual exploratory behavior of these birds. We show that fast-exploring territorial males had higher dominance ranks than did slow-exploring territorial males in two out of three samples, and that dominance related negatively to the distance between the site of observation and the territory. In contrast, fast-exploring nonterritorial juveniles had lower dominance ranks than did slow-exploring nonterritorial juveniles, implying that the relation between dominance and personality is context-dependent in the wild. We discuss how these patterns in dominance can explain earlier reported effects of avian personality on natal dispersal and fitness. Copyright 2004.

Suggested Citation

  • Niels Jeroen Dingemanse & Piet de Goede, 2004. "The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 15(6), pages 1023-1030, November.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:15:y:2004:i:6:p:1023-1030
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arh115
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    Citations

    Citations are extracted by the CitEc Project, subscribe to its RSS feed for this item.
    as


    Cited by:

    1. Lisa J Wallis & Ivaylo B Iotchev & Enikő Kubinyi, 2020. "Assertive, trainable and older dogs are perceived as more dominant in multi-dog households," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 15(1), pages 1-17, January.
    2. Samantha C Patrick & Lucy E Browning, 2011. "Exploration Behaviour Is Not Associated with Chick Provisioning in Great Tits," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 6(10), pages 1-7, October.
    3. Hannah A. Edwards & Terry Burke & Hannah L. Dugdale, 2017. "Repeatable and heritable behavioural variation in a wild cooperative breeder," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 28(3), pages 668-676.
    4. Katherine A. Herborn & Britt J. Heidinger & Lucille Alexander & Kathryn E. Arnold, 2014. "Personality predicts behavioral flexibility in a fluctuating, natural environment," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(6), pages 1374-1379.
    5. Mei-Ling Bai & Lucia Liu Severinghaus & Mark Todd Philippart, 2012. "Mechanisms underlying small-scale partial migration of a subtropical owl," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(1), pages 153-159.
    6. László Zsolt Garamszegi & Gábor Markó & Gábor Herczeg, 2013. "A meta-analysis of correlated behaviors with implications for behavioral syndromes: relationships between particular behavioral traits," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(5), pages 1068-1080.
    7. Christina Rockwell & Pia O. Gabriel & Jeffrey M. Black, 2012. "Bolder, older, and selective: factors of individual-specific foraging behaviors in Steller’s jays," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(3), pages 676-683.

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:15:y:2004:i:6:p:1023-1030. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.